How does aggression affect people




















J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. The neurobiology of aggression and violence. CNS Spectr. Connections between the central nucleus of the amygdala and the midbrain periaqueductal gray: topography and reciprocity. J Comp Neurol. Organization of connections between the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and lateral hypothalamus: a single and double retrograde tracing study in rats.

Brain Struct Funct. Swanson LW. What is the brain? Trends Neurosci. LeDoux J. The amygdala. Curr Biol. The hypothalamus. Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy , Vol. Amsterda : Elsevier Sciences ; : 1 — The integration of stress by the hypothalamus, amygdala and prefrontal cortex: balance between the autonomic nervous system and the neuroendocrine system.

Prog Brain Res. Neurobiology of escalated aggression and violence. J Neurosci. Pleiotropic contributions of nitric oxide to aggressive behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. The role of monoamine oxidase A in aggression: current translational developments and future challenges. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. Citrome L , Volavka J. The psychopharmacology of violence: making sensible decisions. Brief report: Impact of applied behaviour analysis ABA on carer burden and community participation in challenging behaviour: results from a randomised controlled trial.

J Intellect Disabil Res. Treatment moderators of cognitive behavior therapy to reduce aggressive behavior: a meta-analysis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Efficacy and safety of maintenance electroconvulsive therapy for sustaining resolution of severe aggression in a major neurocognitive disorder. BMJ Case Rep. Electroconvulsive therapy for agitation and aggression in dementia: a systematic review. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. Prado-Lima PAS do. Dopamine D2 receptor occupancy and clinical effects.

Treating the violent patient with psychosis or impulsivity utilizing antipsychotic polypharmacy and high-dose monotherapy. Biological treatment of acute agitation or aggression with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder in the inpatient setting.

Ann Clin Psychiatry. Drug-refractory aggression, self-injurious behavior, and severe tantrums in autism spectrum disorders: a chart review study. Efficacy of atypical antipsychotics in the management of acute agitation and aggression in hospitalized patients with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder: results from a systematic review.

Shanghai Arch Psychiatry. Unremitting impulsive aggression in a child with childhood onset schizophrenia and pervasive development disorder-not otherwise specified: the role of stimulants, atypical antipsychotics and mood stabilizers. Fava M. Psychopharmacologic treatment of pathologic aggression. Psychiatr Clin North Am. Baribeau DA , Anagnostou E. An update on medication management of behavioral disorders in autism.

Curr Psychiatry Rep ; 16 3 : Benzodiazepine use and aggressive behaviour: a systematic review. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. Willner P. The neurobiology of aggression: Implications for the pharmacotherapy of aggressive challenging behaviour by people with intellectual disabilities. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. Stigler KA. Psychopharmacologic management of serious behavioral disturbance in ASD. Alpha-2 adrenergic receptor agonists: a review of current clinical applications.

Anesth Prog. Pharmacological and behavioral treatments for aggressive psychiatric inpatients. Hosp Community Psychiatry. A double-blind placebo-controlled study of lithium in hospitalized aggressive children and adolescents with conduct disorder.

Evidence-based recommendations for the treatment of aggression in pediatric patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatr Q. Self-harm, unintentional injury, and suicide in bipolar disorder during maintenance mood stabilizer treatment. JAMA Psychiatry. Clinically relevant doses of methylphenidate significantly occupy norepinephrine transporters in humans in vivo.

Biol Psychiatry. Neurocircuitry underlying the preferential sensitivity of prefrontal catecholamines to low-dose psychostimulants. Pharmacotherapy of aggression in child and adolescent psychiatric disorders. The pharmacological treatment of aggression in children and adolescents with conduct disorder. Do callous-unemotional traits modulate the efficacy of medication? New research. Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol News. Pharmacological management of persistent hostility and aggression in persons with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a systematic review.

J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. The pharmacological management of oppositional behaviour, conduct problems, and aggression in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis.

Part 2: antipsychotics and traditional mood stabilizers. Can J Psychiatry. Toro-Martinez E. Pharmacological treatment of aggressive impulsive behavior. Accessed August 3, Volumetry of the human amygdala—an anatomical study.

Psychiatry Res. What is the amygdala? Johnston JB. Further contributions to the study of the evolution of the forebrain. Organization of projections from the medial nucleus of the amygdala: a PHAL study in the rat. The human amygdaloid complex: a cytologic and histochemical atlas using Nissl, myelin, acetylcholinesterase and nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide phosphate diaphorase staining. Active vs. Learn Mem. Neurobiology of emotion perception I: the neural basis of normal emotion perception.

Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res. Goltz F. Der Hund ohne Grosshirn. Brown S , Shafer E. An investigation into the functions of the occipital an temporal lobes of the monkey's brain. Kluver H , Bucy PC. Am J Physiol. Preliminary analysis of functions of the temporal lobes in monkeys. Arch NeurPsych.

Influence of amygdalectomy on social behavior in monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol. Stereotactic amygdalotomy for convulsive and behavioral disorders.

Long-term follow-up study. Appl Neurophysiol. Stereotaxic amygdalotomy for behavior disorders. Arch Neurol. Balasubramaniam V , Ramamurthi B. Stereotaxic amygdalotomy. Proc Aust Assoc Neurol. Deep temporal stimulation in man.

Stereotaxic amygdalotomy in behavior disorders. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg. Vaernet K , Madsen A. Stereotaxic amygdalotomy and basofrontal tractotomy in psychotics with aggressive behaviour. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. Aggressive behavior violates social boundaries. It can lead to breakdowns in your relationships.

It can be obvious or secretive. Occasional aggressive outbursts are common and even normal in the right circumstances. However, you should speak to your doctor if you experience aggressive behavior frequently or in patterns.

When you engage in aggressive behavior, you may feel irritable and restless. You may feel impulsive. You may find it hard to control your behavior. You might not know which behaviors are socially appropriate. In other cases, you might act aggressively on purpose. For example, you may use aggressive behavior to get revenge or provoke someone. You may also direct aggressive behavior towards yourself. This can help you address it. As an adult, you might act aggressively in response to negative experiences.

For example, you might get aggressive when you feel frustrated. Your aggressive behavior may also be linked to depression, anxiety, PTSD, or other mental health conditions. Many mental health conditions can contribute to aggressive behavior.

For example, these conditions include:. Brain damage can also limit your ability to control aggression. You may experience brain damage as the result of:. Different health conditions contribute to aggression in different ways. For example, if you have autism or bipolar disorder, you might act aggressively when you feel frustrated or unable to speak about your feelings. If you have conduct disorder, you will act aggressively on purpose. Your child might imitate aggressive or violent behavior that they see in their daily life.

Using a novel technique, researchers have been able The researchers hypothesize that a lower channel density may have A new study has tackled Recently published research found Giant Leap Taken in Fighting Antibiotic Print Email Share. Most Popular Stories. Just a Game? Another way to examine neural correlates of cute aggression is to explore reward-related ERP components. The stimulus preceding negativity SPN is a slow wave component that reflects the expectation of reward stimuli Damen and Brunia, The significance of the SPN is typically conceptualized as emotional anticipation Chwilla and Brunia, ; Kotani et al.

The SPN is typically measured after participants make a motor response and before feedback onset in a decision-making task Brunia et al. The SPN is sensitive to reward magnitude and is consistently larger in reward versus no-reward conditions Kotani et al. Though the SPN is typically measured in decision-making tasks, previous research has reported that the SPN can be observed when anticipating affective upcoming stimuli without a task Takeuchi et al.

Findings indicated that there was a larger SPN when anticipating strongly affective pictures when compared to neutral pictures. Given the relevance of the SPN to reward and affective anticipation, we identified this component as potentially relevant to cute aggression.

To our knowledge, no research has evaluated the association between the SPN, cute stimuli, and expressions of aggression. The RewP response is a positive component that peaks ms after rewarding stimuli Miltner et al. Numerous studies have shown that the RewP is elicited by positive feedback Baker and Holroyd, ; Foti et al. Previous research on losses and gains has suggests that the RewP was larger when associated with actual rewards and smaller for non-rewards Carlson et al.

The RewP predicted individual differences among sensitivity levels for rewards when evaluated using both behavioral and self-report measures Bress and Hajcak, Overall, findings suggest that the RewP is a neural correlate of positive and negative reward stimuli for a review, see Proudfit, To date, no research has investigated the relationship between cute images and the RewP.

A second potential mechanism for cute aggression relates to reward anticipation and processing. We hypothesized that expressions of cute aggression might be related to finding stimuli particularly rewarding. As the current study involved passive viewing rather than an overt task , we hypothesized that expressions of cute aggression might relate to SPN amplitude, Reward positivity RewP amplitude, or both.

Finally, we explored the relationship between brain and behavioral ratings using both correlations and mediation models e.

Participants had no history of developmental disabilities or psychiatric conditions and were not taking any medications for psychiatric or neurological conditions as per self-report.

One participant was tested but later excluded because we learned she had a previous psychiatric diagnosis which was unknown at the time of testing. Participants were recruited through the University of California, Riverside subject pool and from on-campus flyers. All participants were over 18 years of age and signed a consent form. The current study had four blocks of trials, each containing different images: more cute baby animals, less cute adult animals, more cute babies, less cute babies.

They identified and validated eight infantile images, and eight images of older animals. The eight images were of the following animal species: elephant, duck, pig, cat, monkey, dog, and rabbit.

These infant photographs in each condition were originally validated in the Sherman et al. Participants did not engage in any overt task. Each participant saw all four blocks of stimuli more cute animals, less cute animals, more cute babies, less cute babies in a random order. Randomization was done using a random number generator prior to each subject.

Within each block, each photograph was shown four times for a total of 32 trials in each block in a pseudo-random order such that no photograph was repeated more than twice in a row.

Participants were told that they would be passively viewing different photographs on the screen, and that they would be filling out questionnaires about each set block of photographs. Between each block, participants were asked to complete behavioral measures indicating how they felt about the pictures they saw. Including time for behavioral ratings between each block, the total duration of the EEG portion of the experiment was approximately 25 min.

Between each block of EEG stimuli, participants were asked to fill out rating scales related to their feelings about each block of pictures. At the top of the rating scale, the following statement was used to help participants understand the meaning and interpretation of the questions:. Sometimes we say things and appear to be more angry than happy, even though we are happy.

We wanted participants to understand that these expressions of aggression are made in the absence of any intent to harm. We thought that if participants believed the point of the research was to understand aggressive impulses made with the intent to harm the cute thing, we would not obtain accurate or representative responses. EEG data were recorded using a Brain Products ActiCHamp system with electrodes located at 32 standard scalp locations of the extended international system. Data was sampled at Hz.

Vertical and horizontal electrooculogram EOG were measured from electrodes located lateral to the outer canthus of each eye and from electrodes located above and below the left eye. Offline, the EEG signals were re-referenced to the average of the two mastoid electrodes and filtered at 30 Hz and 0.

Each trial began with a fixation cross, which remained onscreen for ms. Following the fixation cross, a 3, ms pause occurred to allow the SPN to be measured. Following the pause, images of babies or animals were displayed for 1, ms. The inter-trial interval was varied randomly between and ms. Trials were time locked to the onset of the images of babies or animals. To measure reward anticipation, the baseline period was -3, to -3, ms, and the data was epoched from -3, to ms.

Note that in previous studies, electrodes T5 and T6 were used, and the current study utilized T7 and T8. This is due to differences in electrode placement and layout between electrode caps. To measure emotion and reward processing, the baseline period was to 0 ms, and the data were epoched from to ms. Peak amplitude was detected within the following time window: — ms.

For the RewP, based on previous studies Oumeziane and Foti, mean amplitude was calculated for each condition as the average of frontocentral electrodes Fz, FC1, FC2 between and ms. The RewP was defined as the difference between the more cute and less cute condition for animals and babies separately. Trials containing electrophysiological artifacts were excluded from the averages.

Artifacts were removed via a four-step process. Following inspection, data were epoched and eye blink artifacts were identified using independent component analysis ICA. Individual components were inspected alongside epoched data, and blink components were removed. To remove additional artifacts, we utilized a moving window peak-to-peak procedure in ERPlab Lopez-Calderon and Luck, , with a ms moving window, a ms window step, and a mV voltage threshold.

Participants with less than 10 trials in any condition were excluded from statistical analysis. Our final analyses for reward anticipation SPN included 51 participants two were excluded for having insufficient trials , and final analysis for emotion processing N and reward processing RewP included 49 participants four were excluded for having insufficient trials.

Paired t -tests were used to test for differences in behavioral ratings between conditions: more versus less cute animals and babies. For animals and babies separately, paired-sample t -tests were run on the following behavioral ratings: ratings of cuteness appraisal , cute aggression, being overwhelmed, approach, and caretaking Table 1. Greenhouse-Geisser corrected degrees of freedom are reported to account for violations of sphericity. The Reward positivity RewP is a difference wave calculated from a single electrode cluster by subtracting the less rewarding condition from the more rewarding condition.

The RewP was utilized in mediation models and correlations reported below. To test for correlations between ERP components and behavioral ratings of cute aggression bivariate correlations were conducted in SPSS. Relationships of interest were estimated using bootstrapping procedures, by resampling the data 5, times. Bootstrapping was also used to obtain confidence intervals for mediation effects. Additionally, we tested three serial mediation models using PROCESS Model 6 Hayes, : 1 To test whether the relationship between feelings of caretaking X and expressions of cute aggression Y would be serially mediated by appraisal M 1 and feelings of being overwhelmed M 2 and 2 To test whether the relationship between RewP amplitude X and expressions of cute aggression Y would be serially mediated by appraisal M 1 and feeling overwhelmed M 2.

Ratings for each item are shown for animals in Table 2. Thus, each participant had two difference scores for each of the five items reported in Table 2 e. Independent samples t -tests were run on the difference scores between genders. No significant differences were observed in either the animal or baby conditions for any of the behavioral rating items.

Therefore, Table 2 shows data from all participants. Percentages of participants who responded affirmatively can be found in Table 3. To check for differences between males and females on endorsing these items, Chi-Square tests were run on each of the items with gender as a between subjects variable. TABLE 3. Note that because five correlations were run for each condition e. Therefore, p -values that are below 0. Individuals who reported having squeezed a cute baby exhibited significantly higher ratings of cute aggression in response to more cute babies compared to individuals who reported never having squeezed a cute baby.

To confirm that brain activity in the ms prior to stimulus reflected a reliable SPN, four one-sample t -tests were run against 0 using in all four conditions, collapsed across hemisphere and electrodes. Therefore, because we were unable to confirm that the SPN was reliably elicited, no further statistical tests were conducted with SPN amplitude. When the ANOVA was re-run with gender as a between subjects factor, no significant effects of gender were observed.

Grand averaged waveforms for the N in response to more cute animals black line and less cute animals red line in anterior electrode sites, collapsed across hemisphere. The area between and ms, used for statistical analysis, is outlined with a black box. Bivariate correlations were conducted using the ERP components of interest and behavioral ratings of both cute aggression and dimorphous expression of positive emotions.

For each component N, RewP , three correlations were run: one between brain activity to cute animals and behavioral ratings of cute aggression in response cute animals, a second between brain activity to babies and behavioral ratings of cute aggression in response to babies, and a third between the ERP component of interest and ratings of dimorphous expressions of positive emotions.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000